翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Attached gas-check
・ Attached organization
・ Attached Senior School of Shandong Normal University
・ Attached Support Processor
・ Attachiamenta bonorum
・ Attachie
・ Attachmate
・ Attachment
・ Attachment (law)
・ Attachment disorder
・ Attachment in adults
・ Attachment in children
・ Attachment measures
・ Attachment of earnings
・ Attachment parenting
Attachment theory
・ Attachment theory and psychology of religion
・ Attachment therapy
・ Attachment Unit Interface
・ Attachment-based psychotherapy
・ Attachment-based therapy (children)
・ Attachments (TV series)
・ Attaché
・ Attaché (disambiguation)
・ Attaché Temporaire d'Enseignement et de Recherche
・ Attacin
・ Attack
・ Attack & Release
・ Attack (1956 film)
・ Attack (2015 film)


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Attachment theory : ウィキペディア英語版
Attachment theory

Attachment theory is a psychological model that attempts to describe the dynamics of long-term interpersonal relationships between humans. However, "attachment theory is not formulated as a general theory of relationships. It addresses only a specific facet" (Waters et al. 2005: 81): how human beings respond within relationships when hurt, separated from loved ones, or perceiving a threat.〔Waters, E., Corcoran, D. & Anafarta, M. (2005) 'Attachment, Other Relationships, and the Theory that All Good Things Go Together' Human Development 48:80–84〕 Essentially, attachment depends on the person's ability to develop basic trust in their caregivers and self.〔Levy, Terry M., and Michael Orlans. "Corrective Attachment Therapy." ''Attachment, Trauma, and Healing: Understanding and Treating Attachment Disorder in Children and Families''. 2.nd ed. London: Jessica Kingsley, 2014. 201. Print. ISBN 978-1-84905-888-9〕 In infants, attachment as a motivational and behavioral system directs the child to seek proximity with a familiar caregiver when they are alarmed, with the expectation that they will receive protection and emotional support. John Bowlby believed that the tendency for primate infants to develop attachments to familiar caregivers was the result of evolutionary pressures, since attachment behavior would facilitate the infant's survival in the face of dangers such as predation or exposure to the elements.〔Landa, S., & Duschinsky, R. (2013) "Crittenden's dynamic–maturational model of attachment and adaptation." Review of General Psychology 17.3〕
The most important tenet of attachment theory is that an infant needs to develop a relationship with at least one primary caregiver for the child's successful social and emotional development, and in particular for learning how to effectively regulate their feelings. Fathers or any other individuals, are equally likely to become principal attachment figures if they provide most of the child care and related social interaction.〔Holmes p. 69.〕 In the presence of a sensitive and responsive caregiver, the infant will use the caregiver as a "safe base" from which to explore. It should be recognized that "even sensitive caregivers get it right only about 50 percent of the time. Their communications are either out of synch, or mismatched. There are times when parents feel tired or distracted. The telephone rings or there is breakfast to prepare. In other words, attuned interactions rupture quite frequently. But the hallmark of a sensitive caregiver is that the ruptures are managed and repaired."〔Howe, D. (2011) Attachment across the life course, London: Palgrave〕
Attachments between infants and caregivers form even if this caregiver is not sensitive and responsive in social interactions with them.〔Umemura, T., Jacobvitz, D., Messina, S. & Hazan, N. (2013) 'Do toddlers prefer the primary caregiver or the parent with whom they feel more secure?' Infant Behavior and Development 36(1): 102–114〕 This has important implications. Infants cannot exit unpredictable or insensitive caregiving relationships. Instead they must manage themselves as best they can within such relationships. Research by developmental psychologist Mary Ainsworth in the 1960s and 70s found that children will have different patterns of attachment depending primarily on how they experienced their early caregiving environment. Early patterns of attachment, in turn, shape — but do not determine — the individual's expectations in later relationships.
Four different attachment classifications have been identified in children: secure attachment, anxious-ambivalent attachment, anxious-avoidant attachment, and disorganized attachment. Attachment theory has become the dominant theory used today in the study of infant and toddler behavior and in the fields of infant mental health, treatment of children, and related fields. Secure attachment is when children feel they can rely on their caregivers to attend to their needs of proximity, emotional support and protection. It is considered to be the best attachment style. Separation anxiety is what infants feel when they are separated from their caregivers. Anxious-ambivalent attachment is when the infant feels separation anxiety when separated from his caregiver and does not feel reassured when the caregiver returns to the infant. Anxious-avoidant attachment is when the infant avoids their parents. Disorganized attachment is when there is a lack of attachment behavior.
In the 1980s, the theory was extended to attachment in adults. Attachment applies to adults when adults feel close attachment to their parents and their romantic partners.
==Infant attachment==

Within attachment theory, ''attachment'' means "a biological instinct in which proximity to an attachment figure is sought when the child senses or perceives threat or discomfort. Attachment behaviour anticipates a response by the attachment figure which will remove threat or discomfort".〔Prior & Glaser (2006) Understanding Attachment and Attachment Disorders, London: JKP, p.17〕〔Bowlby, J. (1960) 'Separation Anxiety' ''International Journal of Psychoanalysis'', 41: 89–113〕 Such bonds may be reciprocal between two adults, but between a child and a caregiver these bonds are based on the child's need for safety, security and protection, paramount in infancy and childhood. () begins by noting that organisms at different levels of the phylogenetic scale regulate instinctive behavior in distinct ways, ranging from primitive reflex-like "fixed action patterns" to complex plan hierarchies with subgoals and strong learning components. In the most complex organisms, instinctive behaviors may be "goal-corrected" with continual on-course adjustments (such as a bird of prey adjusting its flight to the movements of the prey). The concept of cybernetically controlled behavioral systems organized as plan hierarchies (Miller, Galanter, and Pribram, 1960) thus came to replace Freud's concept of drive and instinct. Such systems regulate behaviors in ways that need not be rigidly innate, but—depending on the organism—can adapt in greater or lesser degrees to changes in environmental circumstances, provided that these do not deviate too much from the organism's environment of evolutionary adaptedness. Such flexible organisms pay a price, however, because adaptable behavioral systems can more easily be subverted from their optimal path of development. For humans, Bowlby speculates, the environment of evolutionary adaptedness probably resembles that of present-day hunter-gatherer societies for the purpose of survival, and, ultimately, genetic replication.〔 Attachment theory is not an exhaustive description of human relationships, nor is it synonymous with love and affection, although these may indicate that bonds exist.〔Prior and Glaser p. 15.〕
Some infants direct attachment behaviour (proximity seeking) toward more than one attachment figure almost as soon as they start to show discrimination between caregivers; most come to do so during their second year. These figures are arranged hierarchically, with the principal attachment figure at the top.〔Bowlby (1969) 2nd ed. pp. 304–05.〕 The set-goal of the attachment behavioural system is to maintain the accessibility and availability of the attachment figure. "Alarm" is the term used for activation of the attachment behavioural system caused by fear of danger. "Anxiety" is the anticipation or fear of being cut off from the attachment figure. If the figure is unavailable or unresponsive, separation distress occurs.〔Prior and Glaser p. 16.〕 In infants, physical separation can cause anxiety and anger, followed by sadness and despair. By age three or four, physical separation is no longer such a threat to the child's bond with the attachment figure. Threats to security in older children and adults arise from prolonged absence, breakdowns in communication, emotional unavailability, or signs of rejection or abandonment.〔

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Attachment theory」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.